Senses Finding Balance Brendan Borrell

News Scan

Scientific American (April 2009), 300, 24-26 
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0409-24

Senses

Finding Balance

Brendan Borrell

Abstract

Is poor posture control the real cause of motion sickness?

Introduction

To avoid a potentially nasty cleanup, students are deprived of food for four hours before entering the experimental chamber. Inside, they step onto a forcesensing platform and stare at a paper map of the U.S. The walls begin sliding back and forth on a track—a mere 1.8 centimeters with each cycle. At the right frequencies, this movement triggers a tugging sensation that begins somewhere in the brain and mysteriously travels to the belly. But before the full effects of motion sickness set in, the subjects will typically turn away and beg for mercy.
At that point, the students may be thinking that the course credit they will receive for participating in Thomas Stoffregen's sensory funhouse may not be worth it. But for the University of Minnesota psychologist, every student he lures inside is another data point that he believes will overturn the dogma about the cause ofmotion sickness. If he is right, the findings could lead to new ways of identifying people susceptible to motion sickness before they get sick and may provide designers of simulators and video games with ways to keep controllers in the hands of potentially woozy players.
For the past century, scientists have believed that motion sickness derives from a conflict among our senses. Our inner ears contain sensors for both angular motion (the semicircular canals) and linear motion (the otoliths). When these sensors disagree with the information we expect to receive from our eyesand muscles, motion sickness manifests itself. And yet, Stoffregen says, our senses constantly provide different channels of information: redundancy is an essential part of the sensory system, and our brains do not compare our senses in any direct manner. Moreover, because it is impossible to determine which of the conflicting senses is interpreted as being “wrong” by an individual's brain,Stoffregen has branded the conflict theory with the highest-order insult a scientist can muster: unfalsifiable.
Indeed, researchers have long wondered why some individuals and certain classes of people—children and pregnant women—are more susceptible to motion sickness than others. What is more, experiments conducted since the dawn of the Space Age, when NASA wanted to prevent its astronauts from falling ill, can predict who will succumb to motion sickness with only about 30 percent accuracy. Finally, Stoffregen notes the puzzling observation that, whereas people may feel sick on the deck of boat, they rarely get the urge to vomit when fully immersed in water.
Stoffregen instead argues that motion sickness comes from the brain's persistent inability to modulate the body's movements in a challenging environment. Postural instability—the inability to maintain balance—was considered a symptom of motion sickness. Not so, Stoffregen says. Although postural control relies on sensory feedback, motion sickness is really a sign that the motor-control system is going haywire.
His alternative theory, first published in 1991, landed with a dull thud, and his papers garner just a handful of citations each year. But experts have been muttering privately about Stoffregen ever since—and lately some have grudgingly begun to accept him. “It was a very, very different theory,” says Larry Hettinger, a longtime motion sickness researcher now at defense contractor Northrop Grumman. “I clearly remember people thinking, 'This is nonsense, this is crazy foolishness.'”
The growing acceptance of Stoffregen's view has much to do with experiments conducted over the past two decades. Inside the moving room, he has found that volunteerscan significantly reduce motion sickness simply by widening their stance—an observation, he says, not predicted by sensory conflict theory. Students standing with their feet five centimeters apart tend to get motion sickness about 60 percent of the time. Spreading their legs to 30 centimeters increases the stability of the head and torso and decreases the incidence of motion sickness to about 20 percent. Stoffregen says that by monitoring body sway, he can predict the onset of motion sickness with 60 percent accuracy. If swaying is just a symptom of motion sickness, it would be detectable only after participants reported feeling sick.
But the ultimate test of his theory is still in the works. Floating in water, the human body becomes passively stable, and postural control is no longer an issue. If Stoffregen is right, then under such a condition, motion sickness would be impossible, even if subjects were forced to endurethe nausea-inducing camerawork of The Blair Witch Project. Stoffregen just needs to convince NASA—and a dozen amphibious students—to let him use the Neutral Buoyancy Laboratory at the Johnson Space Center in Houston. Unfortunately, the practical applications of that research would be tenuous at best. “I think that water immersion would be a surefire way to prevent motion sickness in orbital flight,” he says. “Sadly, water-filled spacecraft would be so heavy that they would be too expensive to launch.”There's always Dramamine.
The HTML version of this article may not contain all of the images contained in the PDF version of this article, due to copyright issues.
Brendan Borrell is a freelance writer based in New York City

No hay comentarios:

Publicar un comentario

COMENTE SIN RESTRICCIONES PERO ATÉNGASE A SUS CONSECUENCIAS